研究者業績

大河内 浩人

オオコウチ ヒロト  (Hiroto Okouchi)

基本情報

所属
大阪教育大学 総合教育系 教授
学位
修士(学術)(広島大学)
Ph.D(Hiroshima University)
博士(学術)(広島大学)

研究者番号
80223775
J-GLOBAL ID
200901004027200021
researchmap会員ID
5000026037

論文

 84
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    The Psychological Record 74(1) 45-58 2023年12月18日  査読有り筆頭著者最終著者責任著者
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    The Psychological Record 73(4) 513-523 2023年10月16日  査読有り筆頭著者最終著者責任著者
  • Kennon A. Lattal, Hiroto Okouchi
    Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 120(3) 330-343 2023年9月26日  査読有り最終著者
    Abstract Three pigeon dyads were exposed to a two‐component multiple schedule comprised of two tandem variable‐interval 30‐s interresponse time (IRT) > 3‐s schedules in the presence of different stimuli. Pecks to keys by both pigeons of a dyad occurring within 500 ms of one another were required for reinforcement under one tandem schedule (the coordination component), and such coordinated responses were not required under the other (the control component). The terminal link of each schedule ensured that the reinforced coordination response was an IRT > 3 s. Rates of coordinated IRTs > 3 s and total rates of coordinated responses (composed of IRTs > 3 s and IRTs ≤ 3 s) were higher in the coordination components than in either of two different control components in which coordination was not required for reinforcement. This difference in coordinated responses in the presence and absence of the coordination requirement under stimulus control transitorily deteriorated and then was reestablished when the relation between the stimulus and the coordination contingency or its absence was reversed. The results show coordinated responding to function as a discriminated social operant.
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 120(3) 406-415 2023年9月11日  査読有り筆頭著者最終著者責任著者
    Abstract Although hypothetical rewards have been used almost exclusively in human discounting studies, investigations of their validity are limited. The present experiment compared the discounting of monetary reward value by probability across conditions in which the rewards were real, potentially real, and hypothetical. Twenty‐four undergraduates choose between an uncertain large reward and a certain small reward 60 times (trials). In the real and hypothetical reward conditions, the participants made choices with real and hypothetical money, respectively, in every trial. In the potentially real condition, they did so with real money in randomly selected three of the 60 trials and with hypothetical money in the remainder. The log10‐transformed h values of a hyperbolic probability‐discount function and the values of the area under the curve with an ordinal transformation of odds against were higher and lower, respectively, in the potentially real and in the hypothetical reward conditions than in the real reward condition, demonstrating that the probability discounting of hypothetical monetary rewards was larger than that of real rewards. These results suggest that future studies are required to identify why the hypothetical reward procedure overestimates the discounting rates of real rewards.
  • 大河内浩人
    行動分析学研究 37 248-261 2023年4月  査読有り招待有り筆頭著者最終著者責任著者
  • Okouchi, H
    The Psychological Record 72 197-206 2022年6月  査読有り
  • do Carmo, D. C, Costa, C. E, Okouchi, H, Luiz, A
    Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis 48 118-135 2022年6月  査読有り
  • 日野伸子, 高橋登, 大河内浩人
    学校心理学研究 21 35-46 2022年3月  査読有り
  • Hiroto Okouchi, Satoshi Nakamura, Sota Watanabe, Kennon A. Lattal
    Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 116(1) 82-95 2021年7月  査読有り筆頭著者
  • 松本景子, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要総合教育科学 68 87-96 2020年2月  査読有り
  • 宮嵜円, 大河内浩人, 和角輝美子
    大阪教育大学紀要総合教育科学 68 97-107 2020年2月  査読有り
  • 藤田暁, 大河内浩人
    日本歯科技工学会雑誌 41(1) 78-84 2020年1月  査読有り
    歯科技工士国家試験の学説客観試験準備にプログラム学習を導入した。プログラム学習とは、生徒自らのペースで段階的に学習を進められるようにする教育方法である。歯科技工士学科2年生10名が参加した。学説客観試験の全8科目分の客観式2肢、3肢、ならびに4肢択一問題を用意した。2択問題から始め、正答率80%以上で3択問題、4択問題へと選択肢を増やした。不正解問題に対しては教科書などを用いて掲載ページの記入をさせた。また質疑応答時間を設け理解度の確認を行った。従来の講義形式を主とした対策を行った前年度と比較すると、2回目の模擬試験、3回目と4回目の卒業試験においてプログラム学習を行った年度の点数の方が高かった。最後に実施したアンケートでは、プログラム学習について簡単だった、満足した、役に立ったと答えた生徒が多かった。プログラム学習は、学説客観試験対策として有効であることが示唆された。(著者抄録)
  • Lattal, K. A, Okouchi, H
    Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis 45 321-329 2019年12月  査読有り招待有り
  • Okouchi, H, Takafuji, W, Sogawa, Y
    Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis 45 398-416 2019年12月  査読有り
  • 棒谷環, 大河内浩人
    ヒューマン・ケア研究 20 15-24 2019年11月  査読有り
  • 上野妙子, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要総合教育科学 67 69-76 2019年2月  
  • 中村敏, 大河内浩人
    心理学研究 89(4) 396-402 2018年10月  査読有り
  • 十川勇人, 大河内浩人
    行動科学 57 27-32 2018年9月  査読有り
  • 小原漱斗, 大河内浩人
    行動科学 56(1) 11-20 2017年9月  査読有り
  • 大河内浩人
    行動分析学研究 32(1) 61-77 2017年8月  査読有り
  • 棒谷環, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門 65(2) 35-43 2017年2月  
    国内における看護学生の情動知能に関する研究の動向について,先行研究を調査した。24件の文献より,看護学生は(a)情動知能のうち「自己対応」「対人対応」が高い傾向にあり,入学時よりそれらを持ち合わせている,(b)学年が上がるにつれて得点の低下を認める情動知能の項目もある,(c)心の健康と情動知能とは相関がある,ことが実証されていることが明らかとなった。他方,実態を知るための調査にとどまった研究がほとんどであることも明らかとなり,情動知能を高める具体的な方法を構築することが今後の課題であると指摘した。The present article aimed to outline the studies of the emotional intelligence of nursing students in Japan. Major findings of previous 24 studies were summarized as follows: (a) scores of the intra-personal and inter-personal factors on the emotional intelligence were high when the students entered the nursing school, (b) some scores on the emotional intelligence decreased as the school year went up, and (c) scores on the emotional intelligence correlated with those on the mental health. Most of the studies were correlational. It was noted that one of the future research directions might be developing procedures promoting the emotional intelligence of nursing students.
  • Yuka Fujii, Hiroto Okouchi
    Revista Mexicana de Analisis de la Conducta 43(1) 1-19 2017年  査読有り
    The experiment examined effects of instructions on human schedule performance when providers of the instructions were persons other than the experimenter. Pressing a key by one member of each pair of eight undergraduates produced points on a multiple fixed-interval (FI) FI schedule. Another member of the pair was exposed to a multiple fixed-ratio (FR) differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DRL) schedule. After every session, verbal descriptions of how to increase points written by another member of the pair were presented. The final response rates under the multiple FI FI schedule were indistinguishable between the two components in each of four participants although they each received verbal descriptions indicating that the way to increase points with the key was to respond rapidly in one component and to respond slowly in another. Such weak effects of instructions given from persons other than the experimenter were contrasted with stronger effects of instructions given by the experimenter, that is, the final response rates were higher with the instruction to respond rapidly than with the instruction to respond slowly for three of the four participants. This differential effect across the instruction providers was not found in each of four participants who were exposed to the multiple FR DRL schedule.
  • 大河内浩人
    行動分析学研究 31(1) 67-72 2016年8月  査読有り
  • Okouchi, H
    Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis 41 137-154 2015年9月  査読有り
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    PSYCHOLOGICAL RECORD 65(1) 67-75 2015年3月  査読有り
    The present experiment demonstrated a procedure analyzing the behavior of controllers by measuring undergraduates' responses that produced consequences to other undergraduates. One member of each pair of 42 undergraduates, referred to as a learner, was asked to earn points exchangeable for money and told that touching a square shown on the screen of the display monitor may or may not change points. Unlike standard operant experiments, the point change was not determined by any computer program but by responses of another experimentally na < ve undergraduate, referred to as an instructor. Response rates for 15 of 21 learners were higher than those for their yoked partners, who got and lost points independent of their responding. For 17 of 21 instructors, responses to an alternative (S+ key) that produced points to the learners were more than responses to another alternative (S- key) that resulted in the learners' point loss. Each of 16 instructors pressed the S+ key more frequently after the learner responded than after the instructor pressed the S+ or S- key. The mean interval between the learner's response and the instructor's S+ response was shorter than 1 s for each of 14 pairs of instructors and learners. Consistent with the behavioral principle of positive reinforcement, these results suggest that the instructors generally provided positive reinforcers depending on and immediately following the learners' responses.
  • 岩戸優葉, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門 63(2) 29-36 2015年2月  
    8名の大学生は,それぞれ,自分の氏名と顔が相手(被分与者,サクラ)に見られている映像を呈示されている状況で独裁者ゲームを行った(非匿名条件)。7名は自分が誰であるか相手に知られない状況で独裁者ゲームを行った(匿名条件)。さらに17名は,匿名もしくは非匿名条件で,1000円の元手が,自分にではなく相手にある修正独裁者ゲームを行った。匿名性が保証されているときよりも匿名性の低い状況の方が利他的に行動するという過去の実験結果とは異なり,参加者が相手に分与した金額は,匿名条件と非匿名条件との間で差がなかった。Each of eight undergraduates played a dictator game with a confederate (a recipient) when he/she was presented a view that the his/her name and face were being watched by his/her recipient (public condition), whereas other seven did the game when they were informed that their recipient did not know them (anonymous condition). Additional 17 undergraduates played a modified dictator game in which the experimenter allocated 1,000 yen endowment not to the participant (dictator) but to the recipient in the public or anonymous condition. Contrary to previous results showing that individuals behaved more altruistically when they were in less anonymous situations than under guaranteed anonymity, the amount of money allocated from the dictator to the recipient was not different between the public and anonymous conditions.
  • Masanobu Kuwahara, Akio Matsumoto, Hiroto Okouchi, Koichi Ono
    JAPANESE PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 56(2) 139-152 2014年4月  査読有り
    The present study examined whether bidirectional response-response relations could be established without direct reinforcement. In AB training for 12 undergraduates, higher rates of touches to a white circle on the monitor screen (A1) produced two stimuli (B1 and B2) on half of the trials, whereas lower rates (A2) produced the same effect on the other half. Choosing one of the two stimuli was reinforced according to the preceding responding (A1B1 and A2B2). In BC training, touching a stimulus (B1 or B2) produced three white circles lined up horizontally on the screen, after which one of two different response sequences to the circles (left-center-right, C1 or C2, and right-center-left, C2 or C1) were reinforced, depending on the stimulus presented (B1C1 and B2C2). After AB and BC relations were established, 11 of 12 participants showed the emergence of untrained relations (BA, CB, AC, and CA) throughout five test sessions, and the remaining participant showed it in the first four test sessions. These response-response relations were replicated with five other undergraduates and different trained relations.
  • Hiroto Okouchi, Kennon A. Lattal, Akira Sonoda, Taichi Nakamae
    JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 101(2) 275-287 2014年3月  査読有り
    Two experiments were conducted to assess stimulus control and generalization of remote behavioral history effects with humans. Undergraduates first responded frequently under a fixed-ratio (FR) schedule in the presence of one line length (16 mm or 31 mm) and infrequently on a tandem FR 1 differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DRL) schedule when a second line length (31 mm or 16 mm) was present. Next, an FR 1 schedule in effect in the presence of either stimulus produced comparable response rates between the stimuli. Finally, a tandem FR 1 fixed-interval (FI) schedule was in effect under those same stimuli (Experiment 1) or under 12 line lengths ranging from 7 to 40 mm (Experiment 2). In both experiments, responses under the tandem FR 1 FI schedule were frequent in the presence of stimuli previously correlated with the FR schedule and infrequent in the presence of stimuli previously correlated with the tandem FR 1 DRL schedule. Short-lived but systematic generalization gradients were obtained in Experiment 2. These results show that previously established rates of behavior that disappear when the establishing contingencies are changed can subsequently not only reappear when the contingencies change, but are controlled by and generalize across antecedent stimuli.
  • Okouchi, H
    The Psychological Record 63 595-614 2013年7月  査読有り
  • Sonoda, A, Okouchi, H
    The Psychological Record 62 645-661 2012年11月  査読有り
  • 井野内伸彦, 大河内浩人
    心理学ワールド 59 27-28 2012年10月  
  • 張琇涵, 川下愛子, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門 61(1) 33-39 2012年9月  
    近畿日本鉄道の大阪-名古屋間の駅内ホームの上りエスカレーターに乗る客が,右側,左側,真ん中のどこに立ったかを評定した。全体に占める右側に立った人数のパーセンテージは,桔梗が丘駅では90.9,津駅では4.3であった。桔梗が丘駅と津駅の間に位置する久居駅と南が丘駅では,それぞれ,27.7,50.0であった。これらの結果は,桔梗が丘駅から津駅の間でエスカレーターに立つ位置が切り替わる,しかし,切り替わりは悉無的にある地点で生じるのではなく,段階的に生じていることを示唆している。本研究の知見は,文化的随伴性の下での日常行動の一例として考察された。Informal observation suggests that people in Osaka keep right on escalators whereas people in Nagoya keep left. The present study attempted to identify the point in which people switch from keeping right (or left) to left (or right) between the Osaka and Nagoya cities. We observed people stepping on escalators in the Kintetsu stations and rated which side they stood on, left, right, or center. 90.9 % of people stood on the right side of an escalator of the Kikyogaoka station, relatively closer to the Osaka city, whereas only 4.3 % of people stood on the right side at the Tsu station, closer to the Nagoya city. At the Hisai and Minamigaoka stations, located between the Kikyogaoka and Tsu stations, the percentages were 27.7 and 50.0, respectively. These results suggest that between the Kikyogaoka and Tsu stations, people switched the location standing on escalators and that the switch occurred not in an all-or-none fashion but in a more continuous one. The present findings were discussed as an everyday behavior under cultural contingencies.
  • Okouchi, H
    Japanese Psychological Research 54 202-209 2012年5月  査読有り
  • 大河内浩人
    臨床心理学 12(1) 10-15 2012年1月  
  • Mariko Hirai, Hiroto Okouchi, Akio Matsumoto, Kennon A. Lattal
    JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 96(3) 387-415 2011年11月  査読有り
    Undergraduates were exposed to a series of reinforcement schedules: first, to a fixed-ratio (FR) schedule in the presence of one stimulus and to a differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DILL) schedule in the presence of another (multiple FR DRL training), then to a fixed-interval (FI) schedule in the presence of a third stimulus (FI baseline), next to the FI schedule under the stimuli previously correlated with the FR and DRL schedules (multiple FI FI testing), and, finally, to a single session of the multiple FR DRL schedule again (multiple FR DRL testing). Response rates during the multiple FI FI schedule were higher under the former FR stimulus than under the former DRL stimulus. This effect of remote histories was prolonged when either the number of FI-baseline sessions was small or zero, or the time interval between the multiple FR DRL training and the multiple FI FI testing was short. Response rates under these two stimuli converged with continued exposure to the multiple FI FI schedule in most cases, but quickly differentiated when the schedule returned to the multiple FR DRL.
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 91(3) 377-390 2009年5月  査読有り
    The present experiment examined whether a response class was acquired by humans with delayed reinforcement. Eight white circles were presented oil a computer touch screen. If the undergraduates touched two of the eight circles in a specified sequence (i.e., touching first the upper-left circle. then the bottom-left circle), then the touches initiated an unsignaled resetting delay culminating in point delivery. Participants experienced one of three different delays (0 s, 10 s, or 30 s). Rates of the target two-response sequence were higher with delayed reinforcement than with no reinforcement. Terminal rates of the target sequence decreased and postreinforcement pauses increased as a Function of delay duration. Other undergraduates exposed to yoked schedules of response-independent point deliveries failed to acquire the sequence. The results demonstrate that a response class was acquired with delayed reinforcement, extending the generality of this phenomenon found with nonhumans animals to humans.
  • 山内伸作, 大河内浩人
    読書科学 51 14-23 2008年4月  査読有り
  • 大河内浩人
    臨床心理学 8(1) 123-129 2008年1月  
  • 実光由里子, 大河内浩人
    心理学研究 78 269-276 2007年8月  査読有り
  • 鈴井理生, 桑原正修, 大河内浩人
    行動分析学研究 21(2) 76-92 2007年7月  査読有り
    研究の目的 本研究では、ネーミング訓練と比較ノード訓練が刺激等価性の成立に及ぼす効果を検討した。研究計画 3群の群間比較と、2条件の個体内比較を行った。場面 各被験者は実験室の実験装置の前に座り、個別に実験を受けた。被験者 大学生13名。介入(フェイズ1)全被験者に対して線形構造による見本あわせ訓練を行い、5つのメンバーからなる3つの等価クラスが成立したかどうかをテストした。(フェイズ2)被験者をランダムにネーミング訓練群、比較ノード訓練群、統制群に分けた。フェイズ1と異なる刺激セットを用いて、各群に応じた訓練を行った。続いて、フェイズ1と同様に、見本あわせ訓練とテストを行った。行動の指標 正答率と反応潜時を指標とした。結果 フェイズ1では、どの被験者も刺激等価性は成立せず、ノード距離効果がみられた。フェイズ2では、ネーミング訓練群と比較ノード訓練群でノード距離効果のみられない刺激等価性が成立した。統制群では刺激等価性が成立しなかった。結論 比較ノード訓練はネーミング訓練と同等に刺激等価性の成立を促進させた。
  • 寺田眞治, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門 55(2) 45-54 2007年2月  
    現在のわが国では,人間関係を育む機会がいたるところで失われつつある。人間関係能力の育成には,学校教育のカリキュラムの中に計画的に,人間関係トレーニングを組み込んでいくことが必要である。単位制高校の生徒にアサーション授業を試み,その効果を検討した。アサーティブな生徒も含まれる受講生の集団に実施されたトレーニングの効果を標準化された尺度によって検証した結果,関係形成のアサーションの向上が見られた。学校の授業時間内にアサーショントレーニングを組み込むことが人間関係能力の育成に有効である可能性が示唆された。Assertion lessons were given in a credit-system high school. Scores of the relation-formation factor, which is one of the subscales of a standardized scale for assertiveness (Tamase, Ochi, Saino, & Ishikawa, 2001), increased after the lessons. Recently it is said that opportunities to learn how to create human relations have been lacking. The present results suggest that inter-personal relationship training programs, such as an assertion training, being implemented in school is a promising method for fostering socially matured adolescents.
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    Psychological Record 57(2) 241-263 2007年  査読有り
    One group of undergraduates responded under a fixed-ratio (FR) 25 schedule and a second group responded under a differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DRL) 5-s schedule (first history phase). Both groups of subjects were then exposed to a differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior (DRO) 5-s schedule (second history phase), and finally to fixed-interval (Fl) 5-s (Experiment 1), variable-interval (VI) 5-s (Experiment 2), extinction, FR, and DRL (Experiment 3) schedules (history testing phase). Response rates under the extinction and the FR schedule in the history-testing phase were higher for subjects with a history of the FR schedule than for subjects with a DRL history. Such a systematic difference by remote histories was not observed when the FI, VI, and DRL schedules were in effect during testing.
  • 原田健介, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門 55(1) 71-89 2006年9月  
    研究目的:本研究の目的はVIスケジュール下の反応間時間(IRT)に遠隔履歴効果が検出されるかどうかを検討することであった。研究計画:二重履歴条件,単一履歴条件と履歴無し条件の3条件を群間比較した。場面:実験室で個別に行った。被験者:強化スケジュールに関する実験への参加経験のない大学生13名(男子2名,女子11名;年齢19-22歳)。手続き:二重履歴群では,DRL 1秒LH 1秒,DRL 5秒LH 5秒,VI 5秒の順にスケジュールを呈示した。単一履歴群ではDRL 5秒LH 5秒から呈示し,履歴無し群ではVI 5秒スケジュールのみ呈示した。結果:二重履歴群の被験者はVIスケジュールの最終5セッションに,かつてDRL 1秒LH 1秒スケジュールで分化強化された1秒<IRT≦2秒の頻度の増加が確認された。こうした結果は他の条件では観察されなかった。結論:本研究の結果は遠隔履歴効果がVIスケジュール下で生じること,IRTが効果の測度として有効であることを示唆するものである。
  • Okouchi, H, Lattal, K. A
    Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 86 31-42 2006年7月  査読有り
  • 大河内浩人, 松本明生, 桑原正修, 柴崎全弘, 高橋美保
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門 54 115-123 2006年2月  
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    Psychological Record 56(2) 245-257 2006年  査読有り
    The present article discusses how events outside a subject's skin and not accessible to another subject but to an experimenter may contribute to experimental analyses of private events. Of 16 undergraduates, 8, referred to as instructors, first learned conditional discriminations (i.e., B1C1, B2C2, B3C3, and B4C4) in a standard matching-to-sample (MTS) task with the stimuli Bs as the samples and the stimuli Cs as the correct comparisons. Then the other 8 subjects, learners, were exposed to modified MTS trials in which responses of the learners were reinforced or punished not by the experimenter but by the instructors. Conditional discriminations to be established were A1C1, A2C2, A3C3, and A4C4, in which the sample stimuli As were presented simultaneously with the stimuli Bs so that the instructors could not see the As but only the Bs. For 2 of the 8 pairs, the learners learned the AC conditional discriminations from the instructors who were not accessible to the stimuli As. Functionally, private events have been defined by their accessibility rather than their structure (e.g., Skinner, 1953). In this context, therefore, the stimuli As may be characterized as a kind of private events within the present 2 pairs of instructors and learners.
  • Doughty, A. H, Cirino, S, Mayfield, K. H, da Silva, S. P, Okouchi, H, Lattal, K. A
    The Psychological Record 55 315-330 2005年4月  査読有り

MISC

 57
  • Masanobu Kuwahara, Akio Matsumoto, Hiroto Okouchi, Koichi Ono
    JAPANESE PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH 56(2) 139-152 2014年4月  
    The present study examined whether bidirectional response-response relations could be established without direct reinforcement. In AB training for 12 undergraduates, higher rates of touches to a white circle on the monitor screen (A1) produced two stimuli (B1 and B2) on half of the trials, whereas lower rates (A2) produced the same effect on the other half. Choosing one of the two stimuli was reinforced according to the preceding responding (A1B1 and A2B2). In BC training, touching a stimulus (B1 or B2) produced three white circles lined up horizontally on the screen, after which one of two different response sequences to the circles (left-center-right, C1 or C2, and right-center-left, C2 or C1) were reinforced, depending on the stimulus presented (B1C1 and B2C2). After AB and BC relations were established, 11 of 12 participants showed the emergence of untrained relations (BA, CB, AC, and CA) throughout five test sessions, and the remaining participant showed it in the first four test sessions. These response-response relations were replicated with five other undergraduates and different trained relations.
  • Hiroto Okouchi, Kennon A. Lattal, Akira Sonoda, Taichi Nakamae
    JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 101(2) 275-287 2014年3月  
    Two experiments were conducted to assess stimulus control and generalization of remote behavioral history effects with humans. Undergraduates first responded frequently under a fixed-ratio (FR) schedule in the presence of one line length (16 mm or 31 mm) and infrequently on a tandem FR 1 differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DRL) schedule when a second line length (31 mm or 16 mm) was present. Next, an FR 1 schedule in effect in the presence of either stimulus produced comparable response rates between the stimuli. Finally, a tandem FR 1 fixed-interval (FI) schedule was in effect under those same stimuli (Experiment 1) or under 12 line lengths ranging from 7 to 40 mm (Experiment 2). In both experiments, responses under the tandem FR 1 FI schedule were frequent in the presence of stimuli previously correlated with the FR schedule and infrequent in the presence of stimuli previously correlated with the tandem FR 1 DRL schedule. Short-lived but systematic generalization gradients were obtained in Experiment 2. These results show that previously established rates of behavior that disappear when the establishing contingencies are changed can subsequently not only reappear when the contingencies change, but are controlled by and generalize across antecedent stimuli.
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    Psychological Record 63(3) 595-614 2013年6月  
    A matching procedure was used to examine whether humans discriminate fixed-interval (FI) schedules. Fixed-ratio (FR), FI, or differentialreinforcement- of-low-rate (DRL) schedules were in effect during a sample schedule link. Responses meeting the schedule requirement produced a choice link in which each of three circles was presented with a unique color. The choice was reinforced depending on the schedule of the sample link. In Experiment 1, a correct response equal to or higher than 94.4% was obtained for each sample schedule in each of the four undergraduates. Post hoc analyses suggested that time spent with the sample schedule link may have functioned as a discriminative stimulus of the choices. In Experiment 2, after replicating the schedule discrimination with additional four participants, the FI value was changed to equate to the mean time spent in the FR or DRL schedule link. With this modified FI value, the accuracy was deteriorated but eventually recovered, whereas the lengths of time spent in the sample schedule links diverged across the schedules. Previous studies have shown that humans were insensitive to reinforcement schedules, especially to the FI schedule. These results demonstrate that humans discriminated the FI schedule and suggest that human schedule insensitivity is due to other than discriminative properties of the schedules.
  • Sonoda, A, Okouchi, H
    The Psychological Record 62 645-661 2012年11月  査読有り
  • 井野内伸彦, 大河内浩人
    心理学ワールド 59 27-28 2012年10月  

書籍等出版物

 8

講演・口頭発表等

 4

共同研究・競争的資金等の研究課題

 4