Curriculum Vitaes

Hiroto Okouchi

  (大河内 浩人)

Profile Information

Affiliation
Professor, Division of General Education, Osaka Kyoiku University
Degree
修士(学術)(広島大学)
Ph.D(Hiroshima University)
博士(学術)(広島大学)

Researcher number
80223775
J-GLOBAL ID
200901004027200021
researchmap Member ID
5000026037

Papers

 84
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    The Psychological Record, 74(1) 45-58, Dec 18, 2023  Peer-reviewedLead authorLast authorCorresponding author
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    The Psychological Record, 73(4) 513-523, Oct 16, 2023  Peer-reviewedLead authorLast authorCorresponding author
  • Kennon A. Lattal, Hiroto Okouchi
    Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 120(3) 330-343, Sep 26, 2023  Peer-reviewedLast author
    Abstract Three pigeon dyads were exposed to a two‐component multiple schedule comprised of two tandem variable‐interval 30‐s interresponse time (IRT) > 3‐s schedules in the presence of different stimuli. Pecks to keys by both pigeons of a dyad occurring within 500 ms of one another were required for reinforcement under one tandem schedule (the coordination component), and such coordinated responses were not required under the other (the control component). The terminal link of each schedule ensured that the reinforced coordination response was an IRT > 3 s. Rates of coordinated IRTs > 3 s and total rates of coordinated responses (composed of IRTs > 3 s and IRTs ≤ 3 s) were higher in the coordination components than in either of two different control components in which coordination was not required for reinforcement. This difference in coordinated responses in the presence and absence of the coordination requirement under stimulus control transitorily deteriorated and then was reestablished when the relation between the stimulus and the coordination contingency or its absence was reversed. The results show coordinated responding to function as a discriminated social operant.
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 120(3) 406-415, Sep 11, 2023  Peer-reviewedLead authorLast authorCorresponding author
    Abstract Although hypothetical rewards have been used almost exclusively in human discounting studies, investigations of their validity are limited. The present experiment compared the discounting of monetary reward value by probability across conditions in which the rewards were real, potentially real, and hypothetical. Twenty‐four undergraduates choose between an uncertain large reward and a certain small reward 60 times (trials). In the real and hypothetical reward conditions, the participants made choices with real and hypothetical money, respectively, in every trial. In the potentially real condition, they did so with real money in randomly selected three of the 60 trials and with hypothetical money in the remainder. The log10‐transformed h values of a hyperbolic probability‐discount function and the values of the area under the curve with an ordinal transformation of odds against were higher and lower, respectively, in the potentially real and in the hypothetical reward conditions than in the real reward condition, demonstrating that the probability discounting of hypothetical monetary rewards was larger than that of real rewards. These results suggest that future studies are required to identify why the hypothetical reward procedure overestimates the discounting rates of real rewards.
  • 大河内浩人
    行動分析学研究, 37 248-261, Apr, 2023  Peer-reviewedInvitedLead authorLast authorCorresponding author
  • Okouchi, H
    The Psychological Record, 72 197-206, Jun, 2022  Peer-reviewed
  • do Carmo, D. C, Costa, C. E, Okouchi, H, Luiz, A
    Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 48 118-135, Jun, 2022  Peer-reviewed
  • 日野伸子, 高橋登, 大河内浩人
    学校心理学研究, 21 35-46, Mar, 2022  Peer-reviewed
  • Hiroto Okouchi, Satoshi Nakamura, Sota Watanabe, Kennon A. Lattal
    Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 116(1) 82-95, Jul, 2021  Peer-reviewedLead author
  • 松本景子, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要総合教育科学, 68 87-96, Feb, 2020  Peer-reviewed
  • 宮嵜円, 大河内浩人, 和角輝美子
    大阪教育大学紀要総合教育科学, 68 97-107, Feb, 2020  Peer-reviewed
  • 藤田暁, 大河内浩人
    日本歯科技工学会雑誌, 41(1) 78-84, Jan, 2020  Peer-reviewed
  • Lattal, K. A, Okouchi, H
    Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 45 321-329, Dec, 2019  Peer-reviewedInvited
  • Okouchi, H, Takafuji, W, Sogawa, Y
    Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 45 398-416, Dec, 2019  Peer-reviewed
  • 棒谷環, 大河内浩人
    ヒューマン・ケア研究, 20 15-24, Nov, 2019  Peer-reviewed
  • 上野妙子, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要総合教育科学, 67 69-76, Feb, 2019  
  • 中村敏, 大河内浩人
    心理学研究, 89(4) 396-402, Oct, 2018  Peer-reviewed
  • 十川勇人, 大河内浩人
    行動科学, 57 27-32, Sep, 2018  Peer-reviewed
  • 小原漱斗, 大河内浩人
    行動科学, 56(1) 11-20, Sep, 2017  Peer-reviewed
  • 大河内浩人
    行動分析学研究, 32(1) 61-77, Aug, 2017  Peer-reviewed
  • 棒谷環, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門, 65(2) 35-43, Feb, 2017  
  • Yuka Fujii, Hiroto Okouchi
    Revista Mexicana de Analisis de la Conducta, 43(1) 1-19, 2017  Peer-reviewed
    The experiment examined effects of instructions on human schedule performance when providers of the instructions were persons other than the experimenter. Pressing a key by one member of each pair of eight undergraduates produced points on a multiple fixed-interval (FI) FI schedule. Another member of the pair was exposed to a multiple fixed-ratio (FR) differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DRL) schedule. After every session, verbal descriptions of how to increase points written by another member of the pair were presented. The final response rates under the multiple FI FI schedule were indistinguishable between the two components in each of four participants although they each received verbal descriptions indicating that the way to increase points with the key was to respond rapidly in one component and to respond slowly in another. Such weak effects of instructions given from persons other than the experimenter were contrasted with stronger effects of instructions given by the experimenter, that is, the final response rates were higher with the instruction to respond rapidly than with the instruction to respond slowly for three of the four participants. This differential effect across the instruction providers was not found in each of four participants who were exposed to the multiple FR DRL schedule.
  • 大河内浩人
    行動分析学研究, 31(1) 67-72, Aug, 2016  Peer-reviewed
  • Okouchi, H
    Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 41 137-154, Sep, 2015  Peer-reviewed
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    PSYCHOLOGICAL RECORD, 65(1) 67-75, Mar, 2015  Peer-reviewed
    The present experiment demonstrated a procedure analyzing the behavior of controllers by measuring undergraduates' responses that produced consequences to other undergraduates. One member of each pair of 42 undergraduates, referred to as a learner, was asked to earn points exchangeable for money and told that touching a square shown on the screen of the display monitor may or may not change points. Unlike standard operant experiments, the point change was not determined by any computer program but by responses of another experimentally na < ve undergraduate, referred to as an instructor. Response rates for 15 of 21 learners were higher than those for their yoked partners, who got and lost points independent of their responding. For 17 of 21 instructors, responses to an alternative (S+ key) that produced points to the learners were more than responses to another alternative (S- key) that resulted in the learners' point loss. Each of 16 instructors pressed the S+ key more frequently after the learner responded than after the instructor pressed the S+ or S- key. The mean interval between the learner's response and the instructor's S+ response was shorter than 1 s for each of 14 pairs of instructors and learners. Consistent with the behavioral principle of positive reinforcement, these results suggest that the instructors generally provided positive reinforcers depending on and immediately following the learners' responses.
  • 岩戸優葉, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門, 63(2) 29-36, Feb, 2015  
  • Masanobu Kuwahara, Akio Matsumoto, Hiroto Okouchi, Koichi Ono
    JAPANESE PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH, 56(2) 139-152, Apr, 2014  Peer-reviewed
    The present study examined whether bidirectional response-response relations could be established without direct reinforcement. In AB training for 12 undergraduates, higher rates of touches to a white circle on the monitor screen (A1) produced two stimuli (B1 and B2) on half of the trials, whereas lower rates (A2) produced the same effect on the other half. Choosing one of the two stimuli was reinforced according to the preceding responding (A1B1 and A2B2). In BC training, touching a stimulus (B1 or B2) produced three white circles lined up horizontally on the screen, after which one of two different response sequences to the circles (left-center-right, C1 or C2, and right-center-left, C2 or C1) were reinforced, depending on the stimulus presented (B1C1 and B2C2). After AB and BC relations were established, 11 of 12 participants showed the emergence of untrained relations (BA, CB, AC, and CA) throughout five test sessions, and the remaining participant showed it in the first four test sessions. These response-response relations were replicated with five other undergraduates and different trained relations.
  • Hiroto Okouchi, Kennon A. Lattal, Akira Sonoda, Taichi Nakamae
    JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR, 101(2) 275-287, Mar, 2014  Peer-reviewed
    Two experiments were conducted to assess stimulus control and generalization of remote behavioral history effects with humans. Undergraduates first responded frequently under a fixed-ratio (FR) schedule in the presence of one line length (16 mm or 31 mm) and infrequently on a tandem FR 1 differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DRL) schedule when a second line length (31 mm or 16 mm) was present. Next, an FR 1 schedule in effect in the presence of either stimulus produced comparable response rates between the stimuli. Finally, a tandem FR 1 fixed-interval (FI) schedule was in effect under those same stimuli (Experiment 1) or under 12 line lengths ranging from 7 to 40 mm (Experiment 2). In both experiments, responses under the tandem FR 1 FI schedule were frequent in the presence of stimuli previously correlated with the FR schedule and infrequent in the presence of stimuli previously correlated with the tandem FR 1 DRL schedule. Short-lived but systematic generalization gradients were obtained in Experiment 2. These results show that previously established rates of behavior that disappear when the establishing contingencies are changed can subsequently not only reappear when the contingencies change, but are controlled by and generalize across antecedent stimuli.
  • Okouchi, H
    The Psychological Record, 63 595-614, Jul, 2013  Peer-reviewed
  • Sonoda, A, Okouchi, H
    The Psychological Record, 62 645-661, Nov, 2012  Peer-reviewed
  • 井野内伸彦, 大河内浩人
    心理学ワールド, 59 27-28, Oct, 2012  
  • 張琇涵, 川下愛子, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門, 61(1) 33-39, Sep, 2012  
  • Okouchi, H
    Japanese Psychological Research, 54 202-209, May, 2012  Peer-reviewed
  • 大河内浩人
    臨床心理学, 12(1) 10-15, Jan, 2012  
  • Mariko Hirai, Hiroto Okouchi, Akio Matsumoto, Kennon A. Lattal
    JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR, 96(3) 387-415, Nov, 2011  Peer-reviewed
    Undergraduates were exposed to a series of reinforcement schedules: first, to a fixed-ratio (FR) schedule in the presence of one stimulus and to a differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DILL) schedule in the presence of another (multiple FR DRL training), then to a fixed-interval (FI) schedule in the presence of a third stimulus (FI baseline), next to the FI schedule under the stimuli previously correlated with the FR and DRL schedules (multiple FI FI testing), and, finally, to a single session of the multiple FR DRL schedule again (multiple FR DRL testing). Response rates during the multiple FI FI schedule were higher under the former FR stimulus than under the former DRL stimulus. This effect of remote histories was prolonged when either the number of FI-baseline sessions was small or zero, or the time interval between the multiple FR DRL training and the multiple FI FI testing was short. Response rates under these two stimuli converged with continued exposure to the multiple FI FI schedule in most cases, but quickly differentiated when the schedule returned to the multiple FR DRL.
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR, 91(3) 377-390, May, 2009  Peer-reviewed
    The present experiment examined whether a response class was acquired by humans with delayed reinforcement. Eight white circles were presented oil a computer touch screen. If the undergraduates touched two of the eight circles in a specified sequence (i.e., touching first the upper-left circle. then the bottom-left circle), then the touches initiated an unsignaled resetting delay culminating in point delivery. Participants experienced one of three different delays (0 s, 10 s, or 30 s). Rates of the target two-response sequence were higher with delayed reinforcement than with no reinforcement. Terminal rates of the target sequence decreased and postreinforcement pauses increased as a Function of delay duration. Other undergraduates exposed to yoked schedules of response-independent point deliveries failed to acquire the sequence. The results demonstrate that a response class was acquired with delayed reinforcement, extending the generality of this phenomenon found with nonhumans animals to humans.
  • 山内伸作, 大河内浩人
    読書科学, 51 14-23, Apr, 2008  Peer-reviewed
  • 大河内浩人
    臨床心理学, 8(1) 123-129, Jan, 2008  
  • 実光由里子, 大河内浩人
    心理学研究, 78 269-276, Aug, 2007  Peer-reviewed
  • 鈴井理生, 桑原正修, 大河内浩人
    行動分析学研究, 21(2) 76-92, Jul, 2007  Peer-reviewed
    Study objective: Effects of naming training and comparison-as-node training on the establishment of stimulus equivalence were examined. Design: Group comparison and within-subject comparison designs were used. Participants: 13 undergraduates participated. Intervention: In Phase 1, all participants experienced matching-to-sample training under the linear-series training structure, followed by a test examining whether three 5-member equivalent classes had been established. In Phase 2, the participants experienced the same matching-to-sample training and equivalent class test as Phase 1, but with different stimulus sets. Prior to this, participants were given naming training, comparison-as-node training, or no additional training. Measures: Percentage of correct responses and response latency. Results: No participant established stimulus equivalence in Phase 1. In Phase 2, participants who had received naming training or comparison-as-node training established stimulus equivalence, whereas participants without such additional training did not. Conclusion: Prior training on comparison-as-node, as well as on naming, contributed to the establishment of stimulus equivalence.
  • 寺田眞治, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門, 55(2) 45-54, Feb, 2007  
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    Psychological Record, 57(2) 241-263, 2007  Peer-reviewed
    One group of undergraduates responded under a fixed-ratio (FR) 25 schedule and a second group responded under a differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DRL) 5-s schedule (first history phase). Both groups of subjects were then exposed to a differential-reinforcement-of-other-behavior (DRO) 5-s schedule (second history phase), and finally to fixed-interval (Fl) 5-s (Experiment 1), variable-interval (VI) 5-s (Experiment 2), extinction, FR, and DRL (Experiment 3) schedules (history testing phase). Response rates under the extinction and the FR schedule in the history-testing phase were higher for subjects with a history of the FR schedule than for subjects with a DRL history. Such a systematic difference by remote histories was not observed when the FI, VI, and DRL schedules were in effect during testing.
  • 原田健介, 大河内浩人
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門, 55(1) 71-89, Sep, 2006  
    Study objective: The present study examined effects of a remote history of a DRL schedule on interresponse times (IRTs) under a VI schedule with human subjects. Design: Double-history, single-history, and no history conditions were compared across subjects. Setting: The experiment was conducted individually in a room. Subjects: Two male and eleven female undergraduates with no experience of reinforcement experiments, 19 to 22 years old, participated. Procedure: In the doublehistory condition, DRL 1-s with LH 1-s, DRL 5-s with LH 5-s, and VI 5-s schedules were in effect in that order. The single-history subjects were exposed only to the last two schedules, whereas the no-history subjects experienced only the VI 5-s schedule. Results: For all subjects in the double-history condition, IRTs longer than 1s and equal to or shorter than 2s, which had been reinforced differentially under the DRL 1-s with LH 1-s, emitted more frequently during the last five VI 5-s sessions than during the first five. Such a systematic difference was not observed in the single-history and no-history conditions. Conclusion: These results suggest that a schedule history affected remotely on the VI responding, and that the IRT was a useful measure for examining the remote history effects.
  • Okouchi, H, Lattal, K. A
    Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 86 31-42, Jul, 2006  Peer-reviewed
  • 大河内浩人, 松本明生, 桑原正修, 柴崎全弘, 高橋美保
    大阪教育大学紀要第Ⅳ部門, 54 115-123, Feb, 2006  
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    Psychological Record, 56(2) 245-257, 2006  Peer-reviewed
    The present article discusses how events outside a subject's skin and not accessible to another subject but to an experimenter may contribute to experimental analyses of private events. Of 16 undergraduates, 8, referred to as instructors, first learned conditional discriminations (i.e., B1C1, B2C2, B3C3, and B4C4) in a standard matching-to-sample (MTS) task with the stimuli Bs as the samples and the stimuli Cs as the correct comparisons. Then the other 8 subjects, learners, were exposed to modified MTS trials in which responses of the learners were reinforced or punished not by the experimenter but by the instructors. Conditional discriminations to be established were A1C1, A2C2, A3C3, and A4C4, in which the sample stimuli As were presented simultaneously with the stimuli Bs so that the instructors could not see the As but only the Bs. For 2 of the 8 pairs, the learners learned the AC conditional discriminations from the instructors who were not accessible to the stimuli As. Functionally, private events have been defined by their accessibility rather than their structure (e.g., Skinner, 1953). In this context, therefore, the stimuli As may be characterized as a kind of private events within the present 2 pairs of instructors and learners.
  • Doughty, A. H, Cirino, S, Mayfield, K. H, da Silva, S. P, Okouchi, H, Lattal, K. A
    The Psychological Record, 55 315-330, Apr, 2005  Peer-reviewed

Misc.

 57
  • Masanobu Kuwahara, Akio Matsumoto, Hiroto Okouchi, Koichi Ono
    JAPANESE PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH, 56(2) 139-152, Apr, 2014  
    The present study examined whether bidirectional response-response relations could be established without direct reinforcement. In AB training for 12 undergraduates, higher rates of touches to a white circle on the monitor screen (A1) produced two stimuli (B1 and B2) on half of the trials, whereas lower rates (A2) produced the same effect on the other half. Choosing one of the two stimuli was reinforced according to the preceding responding (A1B1 and A2B2). In BC training, touching a stimulus (B1 or B2) produced three white circles lined up horizontally on the screen, after which one of two different response sequences to the circles (left-center-right, C1 or C2, and right-center-left, C2 or C1) were reinforced, depending on the stimulus presented (B1C1 and B2C2). After AB and BC relations were established, 11 of 12 participants showed the emergence of untrained relations (BA, CB, AC, and CA) throughout five test sessions, and the remaining participant showed it in the first four test sessions. These response-response relations were replicated with five other undergraduates and different trained relations.
  • Hiroto Okouchi, Kennon A. Lattal, Akira Sonoda, Taichi Nakamae
    JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR, 101(2) 275-287, Mar, 2014  
    Two experiments were conducted to assess stimulus control and generalization of remote behavioral history effects with humans. Undergraduates first responded frequently under a fixed-ratio (FR) schedule in the presence of one line length (16 mm or 31 mm) and infrequently on a tandem FR 1 differential-reinforcement-of-low-rate (DRL) schedule when a second line length (31 mm or 16 mm) was present. Next, an FR 1 schedule in effect in the presence of either stimulus produced comparable response rates between the stimuli. Finally, a tandem FR 1 fixed-interval (FI) schedule was in effect under those same stimuli (Experiment 1) or under 12 line lengths ranging from 7 to 40 mm (Experiment 2). In both experiments, responses under the tandem FR 1 FI schedule were frequent in the presence of stimuli previously correlated with the FR schedule and infrequent in the presence of stimuli previously correlated with the tandem FR 1 DRL schedule. Short-lived but systematic generalization gradients were obtained in Experiment 2. These results show that previously established rates of behavior that disappear when the establishing contingencies are changed can subsequently not only reappear when the contingencies change, but are controlled by and generalize across antecedent stimuli.
  • Hiroto Okouchi
    Psychological Record, 63(3) 595-614, Jun, 2013  
    A matching procedure was used to examine whether humans discriminate fixed-interval (FI) schedules. Fixed-ratio (FR), FI, or differentialreinforcement- of-low-rate (DRL) schedules were in effect during a sample schedule link. Responses meeting the schedule requirement produced a choice link in which each of three circles was presented with a unique color. The choice was reinforced depending on the schedule of the sample link. In Experiment 1, a correct response equal to or higher than 94.4% was obtained for each sample schedule in each of the four undergraduates. Post hoc analyses suggested that time spent with the sample schedule link may have functioned as a discriminative stimulus of the choices. In Experiment 2, after replicating the schedule discrimination with additional four participants, the FI value was changed to equate to the mean time spent in the FR or DRL schedule link. With this modified FI value, the accuracy was deteriorated but eventually recovered, whereas the lengths of time spent in the sample schedule links diverged across the schedules. Previous studies have shown that humans were insensitive to reinforcement schedules, especially to the FI schedule. These results demonstrate that humans discriminated the FI schedule and suggest that human schedule insensitivity is due to other than discriminative properties of the schedules.
  • Sonoda, A, Okouchi, H
    The Psychological Record, 62 645-661, Nov, 2012  Peer-reviewed
  • 井野内伸彦, 大河内浩人
    心理学ワールド, 59 27-28, Oct, 2012  

Books and Other Publications

 8

Presentations

 4

Research Projects

 4